
Introduction: âMetaphors in Conversational Context: Toward a Connectivity Theory of Metaphor Interpretationâ by David Ritchie
âMetaphors in Conversational Context: Toward a Connectivity Theory of Metaphor Interpretationâ by David Ritchie first appeared in the journal Metaphor and Symbol in 2004 (Volume 19, Issue 4, pages 265â287), although it was published online on November 17, 2009 by Routledge. In this influential article, Ritchie critiques traditional, top-down models of metaphor interpretation and introduces a connectivity theory that emphasizes the importance of conversational context and neural embodiment. Drawing on Sperber and Wilsonâs relevance theory and Clarkâs model of common ground, Ritchie argues that metaphor comprehension emerges through dynamic interactions between the metaphorâs vehicle, topic, and the shared cognitive environment of the speakers. Rather than assuming metaphors have fixed meanings, the article posits that meaning is constructed in context and varies depending on the listenerâs cognitive and conversational background. This approach has been pivotal in literary theory and cognitive linguistics, offering a more flexible, context-sensitive model for understanding metaphor that aligns with how people actually communicate and think.
Summary of âMetaphors in Conversational Context: Toward a Connectivity Theory of Metaphor Interpretationâ by David Ritchie
đ Contextual Interpretation over Fixed Meaning
- Metaphors do not carry fixed meanings; interpretation depends on the common ground shared by communicators.
- âIt is rarely accurate to discuss âthe meaning ofâ a metaphor, as if metaphors must have a single well-specified meaningâ (Ritchie, 2004, p. 265).
- Meaning arises from the interplay between topic, vehicle, and cognitive context in the listenerâs working memory.
đ§ Neural Connectivity and Embodiment
- Interpretation involves neural coactivation and strengthening of associations in working memory.
- âCognitive effect can be thought of in terms of the degree to which processing a communicative act leads to restructuring the network of neural connections in working memoryâ (p. 272).
- Supports an embodied cognition approach aligned with neurological evidence (cf. Kintsch, 1998).
đŹ Common Ground Is Constructed, Not Given
- Based on Clark (1996), common ground is âwhat participants think they share,â not objective shared knowledge (p. 268).
- Effective metaphor comprehension depends on alignment in mutual assumptions, which are often assumed rather than verified.
đ Dynamic Interaction of Topic and Vehicle
- Interpretation occurs through connection-building, not static mapping.
- For example, âMY JOB IS A JAILâ connects the listenerâs context-dependent knowledge of âjobâ with jailâs emotional and situational associations (p. 274).
đŻ Search for Relevance
- Metaphors must achieve cognitive relevanceââmaximum effect with minimum cognitive effortâ (Sperber & Wilson, 1986/1995, p. 15).
- Relevance isnât fixed: It depends on factors like motivation, prior knowledge, and available processing capacity (cf. Petty & Cacioppo, 1981).
đ§ Multiple Contextual Layers
- Communication activates multiple contextual schemas (e.g., relational, environmental, narrative).
- âA single message can alter several of these representations⌠and hence can be relevant in several ways at onceâ (p. 272).
đ Metaphors Extend Through Entailments
- Metaphors such as âSHEEPDOG THIS PROJECTâ create networks of entailments: leadership, protection, herd control (p. 275).
- Deeper metaphorical meaning emerges when secondary attributes resonate with activated schemas in working memory.
âď¸ Ambiguity and Misalignment Are Common
- Metaphors are inherently ambiguous and misunderstandings are routine, especially when participantsâ contexts diverge (p. 279).
- For instance, âMY WIFE IS AN ANCHORâ could mean âsource of stabilityâ or âconstraint,â depending on prior conversational cues.
đ§Š Critique of Conceptual Metaphor Theory & Blending
- Challenges Lakoff & Johnsonâs model for assuming preexisting metaphoric structures.
- Also critiques conceptual blending theory for being âoverly formalâ and cognitively inefficient (p. 284).
- Connectivity model instead emphasizes bottom-up interpretation from context-driven neural activations.
Theoretical Terms/Concepts in âMetaphors in Conversational Context: Toward a Connectivity Theory of Metaphor Interpretationâ by David Ritchie
đ Concept | đ¤ Definition | đŹ Example | đ§ Explanation |
đ§Š Connectivity Theory | A model where metaphor interpretation is based on forming connections between the topic, vehicle, and elements in working memory. | âMY JOB IS A JAILâ | Metaphor is interpreted by linking jail-related ideas (e.g., confinement, punishment) with job-related dissatisfaction already activated in the hearerâs mind. |
đ§ Working Memory | The currently active set of concepts, memories, emotions, and contextual knowledge that influence metaphor interpretation. | Remembering prior job complaints when hearing âMY JOB IS A JAILâ | Working memory serves as a neural workspace where topic-vehicle connections are actively processed. |
đ Common Ground | Shared assumptions and knowledge that communicators believe they have. | Two friends recalling shared travel experiences. | Interpretation depends on what participants think they both know, not actual identical knowledge. |
đ Mutual Cognitive Environment | The set of all facts assumed to be mutually known and accessible during communication. | Both parties knowing itâs raining outside during a chat. | Ritchie critiques this concept as inherently problematic and uncertainâpeople only guess at mutual knowledge. |
đŻ Relevance | A communicative actâs capacity to generate meaningful effect with minimal cognitive effort. | A sarcastic âGreat jobâ after a mistake. | Metaphor interpretation seeks to maximize cognitive payoff by connecting with the most accessible context. |
đ Neural Embodiment | The idea that interpretation involves physical changes in neural connections. | Linking âanchorâ to stability and love. | Understanding a metaphor alters brain activity, strengthening some neural associations and weakening others. |
đŞď¸ Metaphorical Entailments | The extended logical and emotional implications activated by a metaphor. | âSHEEPDOG THIS PROJECTâ â guide team, chase off threats, etc. | Metaphors can restructure broader understanding of roles, tasks, or relationships by activating chained meanings. |
đ§ Interpretive Context | The combination of immediate physical, conversational, and emotional environment. | The tone of âMY WIFE IS AN ANCHORâ during a breakup vs. honeymoon. | Metaphor meaning varies entirely depending on contextual cues at the moment of interpretation. |
đ Ambiguous Metaphor | A metaphor that can be interpreted in multiple ways depending on context. | âANCHORâ = stability or burden. | Ritchie argues metaphors donât have fixed meanings; context determines interpretation dynamically. |
đ§Ź Cognitive Economy | The brainâs tendency to process only whatâs needed to interpret a message. | Not overthinking âThatâs coldâ unless context demands it. | Interpretation usually halts once sufficient meaning is extracted for the current goal. |
Contribution of âMetaphors in Conversational Context: Toward a Connectivity Theory of Metaphor Interpretationâ by David Ritchie to Literary Theory/Theories
đ 1. Reader-Response Theory
âĄď¸ Focus on the readerâs role in meaning-making.
đ Ritchieâs Contribution:
- Emphasizes the interpretive role of individual cognition and memory.
- Meaning is not fixed but constructed differently by each reader/listener based on their cognitive environment.
- âEach metaphor is interpreted in the particular communicative context in which it is encountered, and individual interpretations will not necessarily matchâ (Ritchie, 2004, p. 265).
- This aligns with reader-response theorists like Stanley Fish, who argue that meaning is produced by interpretive communities rather than embedded in the text itself.
đ§ 2. Cognitive Poetics (Cognitive Literary Studies)
âĄď¸ Interdisciplinary theory connecting cognitive science and literary analysis.
đ§Ź Ritchieâs Contribution:
- Advances a neurologically grounded model of metaphor processing.
- Suggests metaphor interpretation involves neural restructuring: ânew neural connections are formed between the network of⌠âvehicleâ and⌠âtopicââ (p. 279).
- Incorporates Kintschâs model of working memory and embodied cognition to explain how metaphor resonates with reader memory, perception, and context.
- His rejection of abstract top-down theories parallels cognitive poeticsâ call for bottom-up experiential processing of texts.
- Contributes to theorists like Peter Stockwell and Reuven Tsur.
đŹ 3. Pragmatics and Relevance Theory
âĄď¸ How meaning is shaped by conversational context and inferencing.
đŁ Ritchieâs Contribution:
- Builds on Sperber and Wilsonâs Relevance Theory and Clarkâs Common Ground.
- Emphasizes that metaphor interpretation is âan interaction of both vehicle and topic with the common groundâ (p. 265).
- Challenges formalist metaphor theories by embedding metaphor in social and discursive practiceâlanguage is never interpreted in isolation.
- Shows that relevance is evaluated dynamically during discourse, depending on effort and payoff (p. 271).
- Adds depth by introducing âworking memoryâ as a cognitive model for tracking these inferential processes.
đ 4. Post-Structuralism / Deconstruction
âĄď¸ Meaning is unstable, deferred, and contextually variable.
đ Ritchieâs Contribution:
- Disputes the idea of âthe meaning of a metaphorâ as fixed or stable (p. 265).
- Demonstrates that metaphors are always situatedâmeaning is contingent, potentially ambiguous, and subject to interpretive slippage (p. 278).
- Example: âMY WIFE IS AN ANCHORâ can imply stability or entrapment based on conversation (p. 277â278).
- Echoes Derridaâs notion of diffĂŠrance, where meaning is always in flux and dependent on deferral and difference.
- Supports post-structuralist critiques of referential certainty in language.
đ 5. Critique of Conceptual Metaphor Theory (Lakoff & Johnson)
âĄď¸ Challenges universalist models of metaphor as conceptual mapping.
đŤ Ritchieâs Contribution:
- While acknowledging CMT (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980), Ritchie argues it presumes preexisting, universal metaphor structures.
- Instead, he proposes context-driven, emergent metaphor interpretation based on dynamic cognitive interactions (p. 284).
- âThe connectivity model⌠does not share the assumption⌠that thematically similar expressions are necessarily expressions of a common underlying conceptual metaphorâ (p. 284).
- Contributes to the pluralist critique of CMT and advances a more relational, situated, and social-cognitive model.
đ§ 6. Contribution to Narrative Theory and Discourse Analysis
âĄď¸ Metaphor as a structuring tool in narrative meaning-making.
đŁď¸ Ritchieâs Contribution:
- Explores how metaphor contributes not just to local meaning but to the overall restructuring of discourse context.
- âThe metaphor strengthens the connections between the speakerâs wife and other facts⌠and lays the foundation for connecting her to aspects of his life yet to be mentionedâ (p. 279).
- Shows that metaphors shape narrative coherence and thematic progression, making it relevant to scholars of storytelling and discourse structure.
đ Summary of Scholarly Value
David Ritchieâs connectivity theory transforms metaphor interpretation from a static, symbolic mapping into a dynamic, embodied, and socially embedded process, enriching:
- đ§ Cognitive Literary Studies
- đŹ Pragmatics & Relevance Theory
- đ Post-structural Discourse Theories
- đ§ Reader-Response Theory
It enables a more nuanced, flexible, and neurologically realistic model of how metaphors generate meaning in contextâand why they often mean different things to different people.
Examples of Critiques Through âMetaphors in Conversational Context: Toward a Connectivity Theory of Metaphor Interpretationâ by David Ritchie
đ Literary Work | đ Key Metaphor | đ§ Connectivity Theory Interpretation | đ§ž Critique Focus |
đ The Great Gatsby by F. Scott Fitzgerald | âBoats against the currentâ | Connects to themes of nostalgia, futility, and emotional resistance in working memory. The âboatâ metaphor is interpreted in the context of Gatsbyâs personal losses and failed aspirations. | Ritchieâs theory highlights how metaphors like this gain resonance through shared cultural narratives (American Dream, progress) that are contextually activated. |
đł King Lear by William Shakespeare | âI am a man / More sinned against than sinningâ | Activates a moral schema in audienceâs working memory. Listeners interpret this metaphor differently depending on their alignment with Learâs plight (e.g., victim or delusional). | Demonstrates how interpretation diverges across audiences due to varying beliefs and emotional contexts, supporting Ritchieâs claim that metaphors lack fixed meaning. |
đĽ Jane Eyre by Charlotte BrontĂŤ | âA ridge of lighted heath, alive, glancing, devouringâ (Describing Berthaâs fire) | Metaphor triggers visceral imagery and danger-related schemata. Context (emotional repression, colonial subtext) activates interpretations of madness, wild femininity, or resistance. | Shows how metaphors shape reader affect and identity interpretation differently based on prior ideological or gender frameworks (cognitive common ground). |
đ°ď¸ Beloved by Toni Morrison | â124 was spiteful. Full of a babyâs venom.â | Here, the house is personified through metaphor. Depending on the readerâs knowledge of slaveryâs trauma, âspitefulâ activates associations of haunting, memory, and violence. | Ritchieâs theory helps explain polysemous metaphor readingsâtrauma, mothering, repressionâall vary based on individual readerâs context and cultural knowledge. |
đ§Š How Ritchieâs Connectivity Theory Enhances Literary Criticism:
- đĄ Contextual Fluidity: Metaphors are interpreted within specific discourse moments, not as fixed conceptual mappings.
- đ§ Cognitive Activation: Each reader brings a unique working memory of prior knowledge, experiences, and emotions to the reading act.
- đ Dynamic Construction: Meaning emerges through neural and cultural connections formed during reading, not retrieved from a static source.
Criticism Against âMetaphors in Conversational Context: Toward a Connectivity Theory of Metaphor Interpretationâ by David Ritchie
đ Overemphasis on Cognitive Flexibility Can Undermine Interpretive Stability
- By asserting that metaphor meanings are always context-dependent and unstable, Ritchie risks undermining shared metaphorical traditions that persist across time and culture.
- Critics might argue this relativism makes it difficult to study metaphor systematically across genres and audiences.
đ§ Neural Basis Is Hypothetical, Not Empirically Verified
- While the theory draws from neuroscience (e.g., Kintsch, Jung-Beeman), Ritchie doesnât provide direct experimental or neurological evidence.
- Claims about âneural connectionsâ in working memory remain theoretical metaphors themselves, lacking measurable validation.
đ Undermines the Role of Authorial Intent
- The connectivity model focuses on reader/listener interpretation but largely ignores the authorâs purposeful metaphor selection.
- This can be problematic in literary contexts where metaphor is used strategically to convey deliberate thematic meaning.
đŹ Displacement of Linguistic Structure and Figurative Form
- By embedding metaphor wholly in discourse and memory contexts, the theory underplays the stylistic and linguistic features of metaphors (e.g., rhyme, rhythm, syntactic parallelism).
- Literary critics may argue that metaphor also works at a formal and aesthetic level, not just cognitive.
đ Limited Scope for Cross-Cultural and Historical Analysis
- The model relies on mutual cognitive environment and shared working memory, which are highly localized and variable.
- This makes it difficult to analyze metaphors across cultures or historical periods, where common ground is not accessible.
đ Conceptual Ambiguity in âConnectivityâ
- The term âconnectivityâ is metaphorically powerful but conceptually vague and underdefined.
- Critics may question how exactly one maps or quantifies âconnectionsâ in working memory without clear operational metrics.
âď¸ Understates the Power of Conventional and Archetypal Metaphors
- Some metaphors (e.g., âlight as truth,â âlife as journeyâ) operate independently of context due to deep cultural embedding.
- Ritchieâs model struggles to explain why certain metaphors recur universally, suggesting that context cannot be the only determinant.
Representative Quotations from âMetaphors in Conversational Context: Toward a Connectivity Theory of Metaphor Interpretationâ by David Ritchie with Explanation
đ Quotation | đĄ Explanation |
âIt is rarely accurate to discuss âthe meaning ofâ a metaphor, as if metaphors must have a single well-specified meaning.â (p. 265) | Ritchie challenges the notion that metaphors carry fixed or universal meanings. Instead, he promotes a view where meaning is constructed in context and varies by listener and situation. |
âInterpretation is always affected by the cognitive environment of the hearer, including immediate context and working memory.â (p. 266) | This emphasizes the listenerâs mental state, prior knowledge, and momentary context as essential to how a metaphor is understood. |
âCommon ground consists of what participants think they shareânot what they actually share.â (p. 268) | Ritchie redefines common ground as perceived mutual knowledge, not objective overlap. This distinction is key to understanding why metaphors sometimes fail or misfire. |
âMetaphor interpretation involves the creation of new neural connections⌠between elements in the hearerâs working memory.â (p. 279) | Central to Ritchieâs connectivity theory, this suggests metaphor functions by activating and restructuring neural links, not by retrieving fixed concepts. |
âRelevance is not a fixed property of messages, but an emergent property of the relationship between message and context.â (p. 272) | Meaning is not embedded in the metaphor itself but emerges from the interaction between the metaphor and the readerâs/listenerâs context. |
âThe metaphor can be relevant in several ways at once, depending on the hearerâs memory and context.â (p. 272) | A metaphor may trigger multiple interpretations, and what becomes salient depends on which associations are active for the listener. |
âThe connectivity model of metaphor comprehension emphasizes the construction of ad hoc connections over mapping of preexisting structures.â (p. 284) | Ritchie contrasts his theory with Co |
Suggested Readings: âMetaphors in Conversational Context: Toward a Connectivity Theory of Metaphor Interpretationâ by David Ritchie
- Loewenberg, Ina. âIdentifying Metaphors.â Foundations of Language, vol. 12, no. 3, 1975, pp. 315â38. JSTOR, http://www.jstor.org/stable/25000846. Accessed 2 June 2025.
- Fienup-Riordan, Ann. âMetaphors of Conversion, Metaphors of Change.â Arctic Anthropology, vol. 34, no. 1, 1997, pp. 102â16. JSTOR, http://www.jstor.org/stable/40316427. Accessed 2 June 2025.
- Penfield, Joyce, and Mary Duru. âProverbs: Metaphors That Teach.â Anthropological Quarterly, vol. 61, no. 3, 1988, pp. 119â28. JSTOR, https://doi.org/10.2307/3317788. Accessed 2 June 2025.
- Ibarretxe-AntuĂąano, Iraide. âVision Metaphors for the Intellect: Are They Really Cross-Linguistic?â Atlantis, vol. 30, no. 1, 2008, pp. 15â33. JSTOR, http://www.jstor.org/stable/41055304. Accessed 2 June 2025.