“Roots of Racism” by Elizabeth Culotta: Summary and Critique

“Roots of Racism” by Elizabeth Culotta, first appeared in Science, Vol. 336, on May 18, 2012, explores the evolutionary and psychological origins of human bias, particularly the tendency to divide the world into ingroups and outgroups.

"Roots of Racism" by Elizabeth Culotta: Summary and Critique
Introduction: “Roots of Racism” by Elizabeth Culotta

“Roots of Racism” by Elizabeth Culotta, first appeared in Science, Vol. 336, on May 18, 2012, explores the evolutionary and psychological origins of human bias, particularly the tendency to divide the world into ingroups and outgroups. Culotta delves into how these biases, often unconscious and deeply ingrained, shape human interactions, foster prejudice, and contribute to social conflicts ranging from everyday discrimination to large-scale violence. Drawing on interdisciplinary research from psychology, anthropology, and evolutionary biology, the article discusses how implicit biases are not necessarily rooted in racial differences per se but rather in an ancient predisposition toward coalitionary thinking. Studies cited in the article demonstrate how even primates display outgroup bias, indicating that such tendencies have deep evolutionary roots. Furthermore, the research highlights the role of environmental and situational factors in amplifying or mitigating these biases, suggesting that while prejudice is an automatic response, it can be overridden through conscious effort and social conditioning. In the broader context of literary theory and cultural studies, Culotta’s work is significant because it provides a scientific foundation for analyzing themes of racial and social division in literature. Her discussion of implicit bias and the construction of “us versus them” narratives aligns with postcolonial theory, critical race studies, and social psychology, offering a lens through which literary texts can be examined in relation to historical and contemporary power structures. This article underscores the importance of interdisciplinary approaches in understanding and deconstructing racism, making it a valuable resource for scholars in both the sciences and the humanities.

Summary of “Roots of Racism” by Elizabeth Culotta

1. The Evolutionary Roots of Prejudice

  • Humans instinctively categorize others into ingroups and outgroups, a tendency that appears to be deeply embedded in our evolutionary history (Culotta, 2012).
  • This unconscious bias influences social interactions and often leads to discrimination before rational thought intervenes: “Your fear spiked in that dark alley before your conscious brain had even registered the young man’s skin color” (p. 825).
  • Research suggests that early human survival depended on group cooperation, which also fostered suspicion toward outsiders.

2. Ingroup Loyalty and Outgroup Hostility

  • Humans show strong favoritism toward their ingroups, even when these groups are arbitrarily defined: “In arbitrarily constructed, meaningless groups with no history, people still think that those in their ingroup are smarter, better, more moral, and more just than members of outgroups” (Sidanius, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 825).
  • Psychological experiments show that people are more likely to trust and help ingroup members while remaining suspicious or hostile toward outsiders.
  • This phenomenon manifests in social conflicts, racial discrimination, and intergroup violence worldwide.

3. The Role of Implicit Bias in Modern Society

  • Studies using the Implicit Associations Test reveal that even people who consciously reject racism still demonstrate implicit biases: “This is in every single one of us, including me” (Banaji, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827).
  • Experiments show that people are more likely to mistake a harmless object (e.g., a phone) for a weapon when held by an outgroup member (p. 827).
  • These biases appear early in life; research on toddlers shows they associate positive emotions with their own race and negative emotions with outgroups (p. 827).

4. Primate Studies Suggest Bias is Evolutionary

  • Research on rhesus macaques shows that even primates display outgroup bias, indicating that this tendency is deeply ingrained in social species (Culotta, 2012, p. 827).
  • Macaques stare longer at outgroup faces and associate them with negative stimuli, reinforcing the idea that intergroup prejudice has evolutionary roots.
  • Harvard psychologist Mahzarin Banaji describes this as “core to our species” (p. 827).

5. Environmental and Social Triggers for Prejudice

  • Context influences how strongly people react to outgroups; threat perception increases bias:
    • “Canadians taking tests in the dark rated Iraqis as less trustworthy and more hostile” (Neuberg & Schaller, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827).
    • Exposure to violent or fearful stimuli (e.g., a horror movie scene) heightens implicit racial bias (p. 827).
  • Fear and uncertainty amplify these biases, leading to tragic real-world consequences such as the Trayvon Martin shooting (p. 827).

6. The “Warrior Male Hypothesis” and Gendered Prejudice

  • Men, more than women, tend to exhibit stronger intergroup bias, likely due to evolutionary pressures favoring male coalition-building (Sidanius, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827).
  • Historically, men were more likely to be involved in intergroup conflicts, shaping biases toward outgroup males as threats (p. 827).
  • Black men in the U.S. experience disproportionate bias in legal and social systems, receiving harsher punishments and facing higher rates of racial profiling (p. 827).

7. Mitigating Bias: Can Prejudice Be Reduced?

  • While implicit biases are deeply ingrained, they are not immutable; researchers explore ways to counteract them:
    • “Making people feel safer can moderate this bias” (Schaller, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827).
    • Exposure to diverse groups and positive interactions can help rewire automatic prejudices.
    • Conscious efforts, such as reading job applications carefully rather than skimming, help counteract bias in decision-making (p. 827).
Theoretical Terms/Concepts in “Roots of Racism” by Elizabeth Culotta
Theoretical Term/ConceptDefinition/ExplanationQuotation/Reference
Implicit BiasUnconscious attitudes or stereotypes that influence behavior toward certain groups.“Your fear spiked in that dark alley before your conscious brain had even registered the young man’s skin color.” (Culotta, 2012, p. 825)
Ingroup FavoritismThe tendency to prefer and prioritize members of one’s own social group over others.“People still think that those in their ingroup are smarter, better, more moral, and more just than members of outgroups.” (Sidanius, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 825)
Outgroup Bias/Outgroup HateThe automatic preference for one’s own group and negative perception of outsiders.“If groups compete for territory or resources, favoring the ingroup necessarily means beating the outgroup and can escalate into hostility.” (Brewer, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 826)
Coalitionary PsychologyThe evolutionary adaptation to form social groups for cooperation and survival, leading to biases toward outsiders.“Our central adaptation is to group living.” (Brewer, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 826)
Arbitrary-Set PrejudicePrejudice based on socially constructed group identities rather than biological differences.“In Sri Lanka, it may be Tamils; in Northern Ireland, Catholics or Protestants; in India, the Untouchables.” (Sidanius, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 826)
Implicit Associations Test (IAT)A psychological test measuring unconscious biases by evaluating response times to word-image pairings.“It takes significantly longer to associate your ingroup with bad things and the outgroup with good things.” (Sidanius, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)
Warrior Male HypothesisThe theory that men evolved to form strong coalitions and display higher levels of intergroup bias due to historical warfare.“Back in the Pleistocene, outgroup males really were dangerous.” (Sidanius, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)
Threat-Detection SystemThe psychological mechanism that exaggerates perceptions of danger from outgroups to avoid potential threats.“Like smoke detectors, the system is designed to give many false alarms rather than miss a true threat.” (Neuberg & Schaller, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)
Stereotype FormationThe cognitive process of assigning generalized characteristics to a group based on limited experiences.“Taiwanese toddlers assumed that a smiling racially ambiguous face was Taiwanese, but a frowning one was white.” (Dunham, Banaji, & colleagues, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)
Primate Outgroup BiasEvidence that even non-human primates categorize individuals into “us” and “them,” suggesting evolutionary roots of bias.“Macaques stared longer at photos of the faces of outgroup members than at ingroup faces.” (Santos et al., cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)
Situational Triggers of PrejudiceEnvironmental factors (e.g., darkness, fear, perceived threat) that increase implicit biases and discrimination.“Canadians taking tests in the dark rated Iraqis as less trustworthy and more hostile.” (Neuberg & Schaller, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)
Intergroup Competition TheoryThe idea that competition for resources or power increases hostility between groups.“More cooperative groups won battles.” (Culotta, 2012, p. 826)
Contribution of “Roots of Racism” by Elizabeth Culotta to Literary Theory/Theories

1. Postcolonial Theory

  • Reinforces the concept of “Othering”: Culotta’s discussion of outgroup bias aligns with postcolonial theorists like Edward Said, who describe how colonial powers create the “Other” to justify dominance.
  • Reference from the article: “Your fear spiked in that dark alley before your conscious brain had even registered the young man’s skin color.” (Culotta, 2012, p. 825)
  • Application to literary texts: Novels like Heart of Darkness (Conrad) and Things Fall Apart (Achebe) depict how colonizers and natives construct each other as the Other, reflecting the automatic biases discussed by Culotta.

2. Critical Race Theory (CRT)

  • Supports the idea that racism is systemic and unconscious: The study of implicit bias in the article reinforces CRT’s argument that racism is not just overt discrimination but is deeply embedded in social and psychological structures.
  • Reference from the article: “It takes significantly longer to associate your ingroup with bad things and the outgroup with good things.” (Sidanius, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)
  • Application to literary texts: Toni Morrison’s Beloved and Ralph Ellison’s Invisible Man explore how systemic racism shapes identity and social interactions, paralleling findings on implicit bias.

3. Psychoanalytic Literary Theory

  • Explains unconscious fear and prejudice: Culotta’s argument about implicit racial fear mirrors Freudian concepts of the unconscious mind shaping behavior.
  • Reference from the article: “These prejudices tap into very ancient parts of our minds, and it’s happening at a very quick, automatic level.” (Schaller, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)
  • Application to literary texts: In Native Son (Richard Wright), Bigger Thomas’s fear-driven responses to white authority reflect deep-seated, unconscious biases on both sides.

4. Structuralism and Semiotics

  • Shows that racial categorization is a constructed system of meaning: The study’s findings on arbitrary group formation align with structuralist views that meaning is not inherent but socially constructed.
  • Reference from the article: “People easily form coalitions based on meaningless traits such as preferring one painter over another.” (Culotta, 2012, p. 825)
  • Application to literary texts: Chinua Achebe’s No Longer at Ease and Nella Larsen’s Passing explore how racial identity is constructed rather than biologically determined.

5. Feminist Literary Theory (Intersectionality)

  • Highlights how racial prejudice disproportionately affects men: The Warrior Male Hypothesis in the article aligns with feminist intersectionality, which examines how race and gender intersect in oppression.
  • Reference from the article: “Black men are more likely to be victims of hate crimes, receive harsher jail sentences for comparable offenses, pay more money for cars—the list goes on and on.” (Sidanius, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)
  • Application to literary texts: The Color Purple (Alice Walker) and Their Eyes Were Watching God (Zora Neale Hurston) explore how racial and gender biases affect Black men and women differently.

6. New Historicism

  • Explains how historical context shapes prejudice: The article’s discussion of intergroup violence supports New Historicist views that racism must be examined in historical and cultural contexts.
  • Reference from the article: “Ethnic distinctions, however, are presumably quite old.” (Boyd, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 826)
  • Application to literary texts: William Faulkner’s Absalom, Absalom! and Harper Lee’s To Kill a Mockingbird reflect historical racism rooted in longstanding social structures.

7. Cognitive Literary Theory

  • Explains how literature can reshape biases: Research in the article suggests that human cognition can override implicit bias, supporting cognitive literary theory’s argument that reading fosters empathy.
  • Reference from the article: “Making people feel safer can moderate this bias.” (Schaller, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)
  • Application to literary texts: Books like To Kill a Mockingbird and The Kite Runner challenge readers’ biases by humanizing marginalized characters.
Examples of Critiques Through “Roots of Racism” by Elizabeth Culotta
Literary WorkCritique Through “Roots of Racism”Relevant Concept from the ArticleQuotation from Culotta (2012)
To Kill a Mockingbird (Harper Lee)The novel exposes implicit racial bias in a legal and social system that favors the white ingroup over Black outgroups. Tom Robinson’s wrongful conviction reflects deeply ingrained prejudices that operate beyond rationality.Implicit Bias & Threat-Detection System“It takes significantly longer to associate your ingroup with bad things and the outgroup with good things.” (Sidanius, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)
Heart of Darkness (Joseph Conrad)The portrayal of Africans as the “Other” aligns with the evolutionary tendency to dehumanize outgroups. Marlow’s shifting perspective on Africa and its inhabitants highlights the psychological mechanisms behind colonial racism.Ingroup vs. Outgroup Bias & Othering“Your fear spiked in that dark alley before your conscious brain had even registered the young man’s skin color.” (Culotta, 2012, p. 825)
Invisible Man (Ralph Ellison)The protagonist’s struggle for identity reflects the impact of social categorization and racial stereotypes. The novel critiques how outgroup prejudice shapes self-perception and societal roles.Arbitrary-Set Prejudice & Stereotype Formation“The greatest prejudice is often aimed at people without an address, such as gypsies and the homeless.” (Fiske, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 826)
Beloved (Toni Morrison)The haunting presence of Beloved symbolizes the lasting trauma of racial oppression. The novel illustrates how historical discrimination continues to affect Black identity and group belonging.Historical Context of Prejudice & Intergroup Competition“Ethnic distinctions, however, are presumably quite old.” (Boyd, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 826)
Criticism Against “Roots of Racism” by Elizabeth Culotta

1. Overemphasis on Evolutionary Determinism

  • The article heavily relies on evolutionary psychology to explain racism, potentially reducing racism to a biological inevitability rather than a complex socio-historical construct.
  • Critics argue that this naturalization of bias risks excusing discrimination as an unchangeable aspect of human nature rather than a product of social, political, and economic structures.
  • Example: The claim that “our central adaptation is to group living” (Brewer, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 826) can be interpreted as downplaying the role of historical and institutional factors in shaping racism.

2. Lack of Intersectionality

  • The article does not fully address how race intersects with other social categories like gender, class, or nationality.
  • While the Warrior Male Hypothesis suggests that men are more targeted by racial prejudice (p. 827), it fails to discuss how women of color experience discrimination differently.
  • This limitation weakens the article’s applicability to feminist and intersectional studies, which emphasize the diverse and layered experiences of oppression.

3. Insufficient Engagement with Sociocultural Theories of Racism

  • Culotta’s analysis lacks engagement with Critical Race Theory (CRT) and Postcolonial Studies, which emphasize racism as a historically constructed and legally reinforced system of power.
  • Scholars like Derrick Bell and Edward Said argue that racism is not just an evolutionary byproduct but a tool of systemic oppression.
  • The article acknowledges that “ethnic distinctions, however, are presumably quite old” (Boyd, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 826), but fails to critically examine how colonialism, capitalism, and law shape modern racial hierarchies.

4. Overgeneralization of Primate Studies

  • The use of rhesus macaque studies to explain human prejudice may be an oversimplification of human social behavior.
  • Critics argue that human prejudice is influenced by language, culture, and ideology, which are absent in primates.
  • Example: While Culotta cites that “macaques stared longer at photos of outgroup members than at ingroup faces” (Santos et al., cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827), human racism involves complex sociopolitical factors that primates do not experience.

5. Insufficient Solutions for Reducing Bias

  • While the article discusses implicit bias and threat-detection systems, it offers limited practical strategies for reducing racism beyond individual cognitive adjustments.
  • The suggestion that “making people feel safer can moderate this bias” (Schaller, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827) is valid but does not address structural racism, policy reforms, or educational interventions.
  • Critics argue that changing individual biases alone will not dismantle systemic racism embedded in laws, institutions, and social practices.
Representative Quotations from “Roots of Racism” by Elizabeth Culotta with Explanation
QuotationExplanation
“Your fear spiked in that dark alley before your conscious brain had even registered the young man’s skin color.” (p. 825)This statement highlights the concept of implicit bias, showing how humans unconsciously categorize others based on perceived group membership, which can lead to irrational fears.
“People easily form coalitions based on meaningless traits such as preferring one painter over another—and then favor others in their ‘group.’” (p. 825)Demonstrates how ingroup bias is not necessarily tied to deep-seated historical conflicts but can emerge in arbitrary and trivial contexts.
“It takes significantly longer to associate your ingroup with bad things and the outgroup with good things.” (Sidanius, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)This reinforces findings from Implicit Association Tests (IAT), where individuals unconsciously associate negative traits with outgroups faster than with their own group.
“Humans are the only animal that cooperates so extensively with nonkin.” (p. 826)Suggests that human social structures are unique in their ability to expand cooperation beyond genetic relatives, making group dynamics complex and flexible.
“One can be expected to be treated more nicely by ingroup members than by outgroups.” (Brewer, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 826)Highlights ingroup favoritism, where people trust and assist their own group more than outsiders, a core concept in social identity theory.
“In the distant past, we had very little experience interacting with people who were physically very different from us.” (Boyd, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 826)Counters racial essentialism by arguing that racial prejudice is a modern phenomenon, not an innate evolutionary adaptation.
“Seeing such apparent bias in primates suggests it is evolutionarily ancient.” (Banaji, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)Connects human prejudice to primate behavior, implying that outgroup bias has been present in social species for millions of years.
“Like smoke detectors, the system is designed to give many false alarms rather than miss a true threat.” (Neuberg & Schaller, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)Explains the threat-detection system, which primes people to be overly cautious around outgroups, even when there is no real danger.
“Black men are more likely to be victims of hate crimes, receive harsher jail sentences for comparable offenses, pay more money for cars—the list goes on and on.” (Sidanius, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)Discusses racial disparities in justice and economics, showing how biases translate into real-world discrimination.
“Making people feel safer can moderate this bias.” (Schaller, cited in Culotta, 2012, p. 827)Suggests that reducing fear and insecurity in society can help decrease implicit racial biases, providing a potential solution to intergroup prejudice.
Suggested Readings: “Roots of Racism” by Elizabeth Culotta
  1. Culotta, Elizabeth. “Roots of racism.” (2012): 825-827.
  2. CULOTTA, ELIZABETH. “ROOTS OF RACISM.” Science, vol. 336, no. 6083, 2012, pp. 825–27. JSTOR, http://www.jstor.org/stable/41584836. Accessed 18 Mar. 2025.
  3. RIDDIHOUGH, GUY, et al. “INTRODUCTION: HUMAN CONFLICT: WINNING THE PEACE.” Science, vol. 336, no. 6083, 2012, pp. 819–819. JSTOR, http://www.jstor.org/stable/41584833. Accessed 18 Mar. 2025.
  4. “NEWS OF THE WEEK.” Science, vol. 341, no. 6150, 2013, pp. 1048–49. JSTOR, http://www.jstor.org/stable/42619287. Accessed 18 Mar. 2025.

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *